READ

Just like bats, humans can use echolocation

It sounds amazing, but we can all learn to use sound to detect our surroundings, just like bats or dolphins. No eyes required.
Karl Gruber
Karl Gruber
Freelance Science Writer
Just like bats, humans can use echolocation

Have you ever heard the expression “as blind as a bat”? Well, it’s not true. Bats can actually see as well or even better than humans.

In the dead of the night, however, eyes are of little use. Yet some bats go out every night and hunt for insects. They rely on the echoes made by sounds that bounce from their insect prey. In the brain of these bats, the echoes form a spatial representation of where the insect is flying about.

This ability is called echolocation, and it is well known in batstoothed whalesdolphins and some species of birds and shrews. It allows them to find prey or learn about their environment when eyes are not so useful.

It works like this. A bat sends a sound wave, and when the wave runs into an object, it bounces back to the sender. The longer this echo takes to reach the sender, the further away the object. Once the echo reaches the bat, the animal’s brain is able to decode it into useful information, such as the shape of the object and how far away it is.

Echolocation in bats

Video credit: SciToons
Echolocation in bats

But did you know that humans, too, are capable of using echolocation? “Even children can learn by themselves to echolocate”, says Lore Thaler, a Professor of Psychology at the University of Durham in the UK.

The science behind being bat-men

Much like dolphins or bats, a human echolocator generates sharp clicking sounds with their tongue. “They are made by pressing the tongue against the soft palate [roof of the mouth] and then quickly pulling the tongue down. This creates a vacuum. This vacuum then ‘pops’, and this creates the ’click’ sound,” says Lore.

This sound will bounce back from any object it encounters as an echo, carrying information about the object it encountered, just like with bats. Depending on your echolocation skills, this echo can provide different information about the object.

“Recent research has shown that echolocation can provide rather a lot of detail about silent objects, such as their shape, size, distance and the material they are made of,” says Andrew Kolarik, a researcher at the Vision and Eye Research Unit, Postgraduate Medical Institute of Anglia Ruskin University, UK.

Blind man using echolocation

Video credit: BBC Earth
Blind man using echolocation

Seeing with sound

But what can you actually ‘see’? According to Andrew, an expert echolocator is actually able to get some sort of visual representation of the world. “A very rough visual analogy would be as if seeing the world through a blurry filter or through thick fog, where you can see the objects but it is hard to clearly make out the fine detail and the edges, and the visibility is just down to a short distance,” he says.

However, Lore warns not to get your expectations too high. We won’t be able to see the world with sound like with our eyes.

“You are working with sound, which is just fundamentally different from vision. Take glass as an example. Visually, you can see through it, it is perfectly transparent, but for echolocation, it might as well be a solid wall. Alternatively, a fleece blanket is invisible to echolocation, but obviously you can see it,” Lore says.

View Larger

Visually, you can see through glass, but with echolocation, it might as well be a solid wall

View Larger

A fleece blanket is invisible to echolocation, even though you can see it visually

In a recent study, Lore found even further details about the inner workings of human echolocation.

We know bats and other animals adjust the sounds they make when their environment changes. But so far, it is unknown if humans can do this too. In her new study, Lore tested that very question and found that, indeed, people can adjust the sounds they make to better fit their environment.

“Another new finding is that we also measured the echoes that people could perceive. And we found that the echoes that the expert echolocators could perceive were very faint. Indeed, based on previous research, people would have said that it should have been impossible for them to perceive these echoes. But obviously, our echolocators were well up to the task!” Lore says.

Can anyone learn echolocation?

The good news is that anyone can learn this ability, blind or sighted.

Lore says that echolocation requires probably as much commitment as developing expert long cane skills.

Lore acknowledges that, while there are many people who have some basic echolocating skills, there are only a few who really master this ability. “I do not know how many people use click-based echolocation at very high skill level, but I am personally acquainted with 14. This is from all over the world.”

Why so few people? Lore thinks it is due to a lack of knowledge about this ability. “For example, blind people may not know about echolocation so just do not consider it. Or when a blind child or adult uses it spontaneously, they might be discouraged by well meaning sighted people because the sighted folks have no clue and they think it is odd,” she says.

Can echolocation replace the long cane or guide dog?

Not quite. Even for expert users who are blind, echolocation doesn’t provide all the information they need to walk about. Blind people, for example, never rely exclusively on echolocation, Lore explains. They use echolocation along with a cane or a guide dog.

“The benefit of echolocation is not to detect obstacles on the ground or holes or drops. In fact, for that purpose, it is quite useless because the ground itself is one giant reflector. Echolocation is useful for things at head level, to detect targets that are far away and to orient oneself.”

View Larger

Most blind people who use echolocation, use it along with a cane or guide dog

Most blind people who use echolocation, use it along with a cane or guide dog

A bat-brain?

There is also a lot of variability in the capacity of learning this ability, and it is not clear what is driving this variability. “One of the issues in current echolocation research is finding out why some people are better at echolocating than others, whether they use a better strategy to hear changes in loudness or sound quality from echoes, for example, or how they develop enhanced sensitivity to sound echoes,” says Andrew.

Boy uses echolocation

Video credit: MyJoyOnline TV
Boy uses echolocation

But expert echolocators seem to have developed some special abilities at the level of their brains. “There is evidence that brain areas that process vision in sighted people become recruited to process sound instead for blind people, and this might contribute to the enhanced echolocation abilities that have been reported for some blind people,” says Andrew.

Karl Gruber
About the author
Karl Gruber
Karl is an Evolutionary Biologist and a science communicator, passionate about the beauty behind science.
View articles
Karl is an Evolutionary Biologist and a science communicator, passionate about the beauty behind science.
View articles

NEXT ARTICLE

We've got chemistry, let's take it to the next level!

Get the latest WA science news delivered to your inbox, every fortnight.

Republish

Creative Commons Logo

Republishing our content

We want our stories to be shared and seen by as many people as possible.

Therefore, unless it says otherwise, copyright on the stories on Particle belongs to Scitech and they are published under a Creative Commons Attribution-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.

This allows you to republish our articles online or in print for free. You just need to credit us and link to us, and you can’t edit our material or sell it separately.

Using the ‘republish’ button on our website is the easiest way to meet our guidelines.

Guidelines

You cannot edit the article.

When republishing, you have to credit our authors, ideally in the byline. You have to credit Particle with a link back to the original publication on Particle.

If you’re republishing online, you must use our pageview counter, link to us and include links from our story. Our page view counter is a small pixel-ping (invisible to the eye) that allows us to know when our content is republished. It’s a condition of our guidelines that you include our counter. If you use the ‘republish’ then you’ll capture our page counter.

If you’re republishing in print, please email us to let us so we know about it (we get very proud to see our work republished) and you must include the Particle logo next to the credits. Download logo here.

If you wish to republish all our stories, please contact us directly to discuss this opportunity.

Images

Most of the images used on Particle are copyright of the photographer who made them.

It is your responsibility to confirm that you’re licensed to republish images in our articles.

Video

All Particle videos can be accessed through YouTube under the Standard YouTube Licence.

The Standard YouTube licence

  1. This licence is ‘All Rights Reserved’, granting provisions for YouTube to display the content, and YouTube’s visitors to stream the content. This means that the content may be streamed from YouTube but specifically forbids downloading, adaptation, and redistribution, except where otherwise licensed. When uploading your content to YouTube it will automatically use the Standard YouTube licence. You can check this by clicking on Advanced Settings and looking at the dropdown box ‘License and rights ownership’.
  2. When a user is uploading a video he has license options that he can choose from. The first option is “standard YouTube License” which means that you grant the broadcasting rights to YouTube. This essentially means that your video can only be accessed from YouTube for watching purpose and cannot be reproduced or distributed in any other form without your consent.

Contact

For more information about using our content, email us: particle@scitech.org.au

Copy this HTML into your CMS
Press Ctrl+C to copy